Saturday, March 4, 2023

Consequences

6. Consequences

Humanity finds itself standing between two extremes. On the one hand, there has been no other time in history when humans have experienced so much benefit from the fruits of their transformation. For the vast majority of us, we lack essentially nothing in the way of physical and biological needs and comforts. We have an abundance of food and drink to sustain and to nourish us, so much so that we usually can luxuriate in the broad range of tastes and smells available to our plate and cup. We enjoy nearly complete safety from predators and from the elements, living in dwellings that not only provide us with adequate protection but also give us pleasure and a fulfilling sense of family and community. We engage in sex usually at our leisure and mostly for the sheer delight of it, able to plan procreation for when it is the most convenient. And nearly one hundred percent of our children survive into adulthood, experiencing upbringings full of education and enrichment, eyeing a future that foreseeably will be even better than the one we experience now. We travel with ease, even to the far reaches of the planet. We experience health and lifespans previously unheard of. Compared to the lives of the earliest of Homo sapiens, and compared even to the lives of our ancestors from just a few hundred years ago, we live in a near paradise. It washes over us so thoroughly we can easily take it for granted.

In addition, we also live in an age where the level of knowledge and understanding regarding the surrounding environment has reached a nearly unimaginable depth and scope. Quantum theory, genetics, artificial intelligence—we seem to be on the verge of obtaining the keys to the entire kingdom, and it is entirely appropriate to think that the universe is somehow coming into consciousness entirely through human means. It is an amazing time to be part of human existence.

But on the other hand, the accelerating pace of change brought forth by the human transformation, along with the immense and growing power of that change—a power that now overwhelms any form of biological defense—this has put humanity into the most precarious of situations. We stand on the precipice of irreversibly ruining the climate for ourselves and for all the other life forms on the planet. We have brought to extinction such a large number of species that we threaten to destroy the balance of life itself. And we have developed and deployed so many weapons of mass destruction that we now have little choice but to merely hope that someone does not pull the decisive trigger and blow the whole thing up. I would liken humanity’s current circumstance to that of a teenager driving a souped-up car on county roads, thrilled by the speed, thrilled by the rushing wind, thrilled by the music blaring from the stereo, but racing faster and faster around every bend and swerving more and more erratically from lane to lane. How much longer before careening out of control?

I believe humanity has come to this precarious situation in large part because humanity is lacking two useful pieces of information. One, as claimed at the beginning of this essay, humanity does not as yet understand itself, it does not yet have a grasp on what has caused, and continues to cause, the transformation that has brought this species to its current place. And two, humanity cannot as yet envisage where this transformation might be heading, or what could be its purpose. We are moving ever faster but without knowing why and without knowing which direction we might take.

The value of accurate knowledge and understanding is that it untangles difficulties and provides useful signposts, allowing us to proceed forward with greater control and mastery, with a greater likelihood of taking future actions that will be constructive instead of destructive. Humanity has seen the benefits of this kind of knowledge and understanding in so many different areas—physics, mathematics, engineering, medicine—and if we were to gain a similar level of knowledge and understanding regarding humanity’s history and humanity’s endeavors, we might come to find that we can proceed forward as a species with greater assurance and much less recklessness, maintaining all the benefits of the human transformation while minimizing the risks.


One place to begin is with a greater awareness—a greater degree of collective consciousness—regarding the dual source of influence that underlies modern humanity. As has been described and emphasized previously, modern humans are to be characterized as being double origined, first as pure animals and then more recently as organisms responsive to the impact of artificial construction. But few humans actually sense or are aware of this dual origin. This may be due in part to the fact that the species has been extremely successful in blending its original influences, giving the overall impression that human perception and behavior operates as a cohesive whole. For example, this blending can be observed in almost every government and corporate institution. These institutions are typically formulated around known patterns of structure, rule and design, and there is a certain degree of objective logic often guiding how these institutions are organized and run. But they are not machines. Weaved among this disinterested structure can be found a broad assortment of activities, protocols, and conventions that are clearly derived more directly from humanity’s tribal origins. For instance, there is the totemic importance of the org chart, crucial for knowing who controls what piece of turf and who is to be giving deference to whom. There is the steady hum of gossip around every corner and throughout every channel of communication. There are the countless meetings, even when there is nothing of importance to discuss. And then there is the aphorism about how one manages to get ahead, namely that it is not so much what you know as it is who you know. These institutions, when stripped down to the motivations that undergird them and allow them to function, can be seen as microcosms of the broad human talent for blending both biological and non-biological influences, of displaying what might be described as an effective mixture of autistic and non-autistic traits.

But I believe the biggest reason that humans are not generally aware of the dual origin of their nature is that they have convinced themselves that human intelligence is innate, that is to say, that human intelligence is predominantly neurological and biological. Thus intelligence, the core characteristic underlying the human transformation, is categorized in the minds of almost everyone as being similar to the rest of the human instincts. We think we have evolved our intelligence, instead of having built it, and thus we regard our modern perceptions and behaviors as simply a natural extension of our biological and animal selves. But this is a fundamental mistake, a deep misapprehension of who we are and how we operate. It is a self-deluding myth.

The danger of this myth is that it obscures the inherent tension that exists between the two different sources of human influence, between on the one hand the animal aspect of humanity, and on the other hand the constructed aspect of humanity. Despite this species’ effectiveness at blending these two aspects when necessary, it still needs to be recognized that in general these two aspects do not always play so well together. They come from entirely different histories and possess entirely different characteristics, and they pull this species in opposite directions. This in turn creates conflict, confusion, turmoil and obstacles to human progress, problems that will not get resolved by misunderstanding what has given rise to them.

The animal aspect of humanity is of course deeply ingrained, and also ever present, because even in the modern era humans must respond to their biological needs and demands. The key word to this aspect is immediacy, everything operates in the here and now, and the morality of this aspect is the morality of survival and procreation. But if this seems a bit too brutish and selfish, note also that the animal aspect of humanity has provided much of the vitality spurring this species into productive action. How much artificial construction has been instigated by a personal desire for immediate or near-term gain.

The constructed aspect of humanity, more recent and also constantly shifting, faces the daunting challenge of having to overcome entropy, of having to marshal and to infuse additional amounts of energy and complexity into the existing environment. The key word here is expansiveness, an ever enlarging engagement with time and space. And the morality of this aspect centers around the need to contain the natural urge towards immediacy, a corralling of the beast within. But if this seems a bit too onerous, take another look at the cornucopia of benefit that has been thereby gained, including the most fundamental benefit, that of human freedom. Ask yourself, would we really want to return to the circumstances of being pure animal.

This ongoing tension between these two different aspects of humanity might seem at first to be equally and well met, but this is also something of a myth. Over the course of human history, one of these aspects has been gaining steadily in ascendency, while the other aspect has been scrambling to retain its relevance. Before the out-of-Africa migration, and even for some time thereafter, the animal aspect of humanity continued to reign supreme. Human life then was still mostly a battle for survival and procreation, even when increasingly aided by the growing influence of artificial construction. But eventually, the constructed aspect of humanity began to take greater and greater control, and today most humans live lives dominated by the artificial construction existing all around them, with their animal selves being little more than appeased. This is a result that seems to be acceptable to many, but it is also a result that seems to be disturbing to some, and I believe a good deal of the pushback against human progress is motivated by a genuine and felt reluctance to let go of our animal selves.

And at any rate, are we even aware of these aspects that are driving our preferences and desires, are we individually and collectively conscious of the history that has forged us into the modern humans we are today? Do we as yet understand ourselves?


As challenging as it can be to understand ourselves today, it is even more difficult to foresee where it is that we might be heading. We could ask, does the human transformation have a purpose or an ultimate goal? Using the context and perspective of Big History, we might ask the same question about all the previous thresholds. The Big Bang, the formation of the chemical elements, the coming together of galactic systems, the origination of life—did these moments have a purpose or an ultimate goal, or were they instead merely a sequence of events connected benignly from one to another? The thing is, these previous events from Big History seem to us to have been predetermined, whereas we also feel that humans today have the freedom to make choices. And indeed, from the modern perspective, there are many different directions this species might take:

  • We could mindlessly slide into eventual destruction. Much like the teenager driving the souped-up car on county roads, we could just simply enjoy the ride until it finally comes to an end.

  • Noting the harm we are doing to the planet and to the other forms of life on Earth, we could magnanimously declare ourselves to be a cancer upon this planet and voluntarily extinct ourselves. This would give Earth the opportunity to heal and to return to its previous evolutionary state.

  • Noting the fragility of biology against the power of artificial construction, we could pursue the possibility of non-biological life, employing gained knowledge in physics, robotics, artificial intelligence and the like to create sentient entities more hardy than ourselves. These entities could then continue the process of accruing artificial construction, safe from the risks that come with biological entanglement.

  • We could continue in much the same way as we are doing right now, but with greater awareness and a deeper understanding of what has brought us to this place. This would include appreciation and respect for the needs of our fellow species and for the contributions of autistic individuals.

I admit to being biased, but my personal preference is for the latter choice. I cannot see the advantage of either an eventual destruction or of a voluntary return to an evolutionary past. These paths go backwards in time, to circumstances this universe has already known. I would much rather take the opportunity to experience something new. Plus I am struck by how the depth and breadth of recent understanding—the expansion of knowledge that ranges all the way back to the beginning of time itself, and all the way across the far reaches of space, and all the way down to the most minute of subatomic particles—I am struck by how this deep and growing awareness has the character of the universe coming to consciousness about itself, an occurrence I believe needs to be respected. And finally, I cannot accept the placing of this responsibility into the care of a non-biological entity—I worry there would not be enough vitality to keep the pursuit going and not be enough awe in the presence of its more sublime results.

The human transformation has put our fate into our own hands. This was not always the case. We were once entirely determined and constrained by evolution’s rules and bounds, we were not the master of our own destiny. But humans today enjoy freedom from evolutionary constraint, and they have gained the power of artificial construction, and they have the ability to increase their own intelligence. Thus we humans, whether we like it or not, we are now responsible for ourselves and for our decisions, from which we will reap the inevitable consequences. I believe there is good reason to maintain hope and cheer. By coming to a greater understanding of ourselves and by taking responsibility for our future, we can continue to experience the splendor of that sentence with which this essay began:

The human species, our species, is extraordinary.

Shedding Light on the Riddle

5. Shedding Light on the Riddle

Several hundred thousand years ago, the dynamic of biological life on Earth would have had a familiar and predictable quality to it. All organisms were earnestly engaged in the struggle for survival and procreation, as had been the case for many hundreds of millions of years, and other than the usually slow-moving drift afforded by geological change and genetic alteration, each species would have found itself proceeding with a remarkable similarity and an unyielding regularity. Birth and death, eating and drinking, fighting and fleeing, procreating and nurturing—day after day, generation after generation, millennium after millennium. Much like today’s TV nature shows, the plot line was always the same. Caught in a vice grip of evolutionary constraint, each member of each species remained locked inside the same general set of rigid behaviors, behaviors absolutely essential for biological continuation, but also utterly tyrannical towards any alternative.

At the heart of these rigid behaviors was to be found a set of sensory and perceptual characteristics just as constrained as the activities they engendered. Each organism was keenly focused on those evolutionarily essential features to be found in the surrounding environment—food, water, predators, rivals, sexual targets—and this intense sensory focus included the notion of conspecific perception, the tendency for each organism to have an enhanced and preferential awareness for the other members of its own species. The upshot of these sensory and perceptual characteristics was a constant reinforcement of a universal consistency. Cognizant of only those environmental features satisfying biological demand, and taking its cues from and copying the behaviors of the other similarly constrained members of the species population, each organism was experiencing its world in nearly the exact same way. Each organism was perceiving its surrounding environment through the same biologically filtered lens, a lens helping to enforce the strict regularity and unrelenting continuity than can be observed across nearly every animal species. And note what was not being perceived. The non-biological structure contained in the surrounding environment, those many instances of symmetry, pattern, repetition, etc.—the regulative impact of gravity, the recurrent cycles of celestial bodies, the logical causation of meteorological events—it would appear that these many instances of non-biological structure were seldom reaching any organism’s ken, certainly not to the degree to have any significant impact on either organism or species behavior. Thus whatever useful information the non-biological structural world might have had to impart to biological life on Earth, that information was remaining entirely shrouded within an undiscerned sensory background.

It is important to remember that several hundred thousand years ago, these statements would have applied just as equally to humans as to every other animal species. For nearly seven million years by then, the hominin lines had been living a purely animal existence, with no indication their behavioral or perceptual experience was fundamentally different from that of the other creatures. Biologically captivated, and corralled into a generational constancy via conspecific perception, humans found themselves tightly ensnared inside a familiar cycle. Birth and death, eating and drinking, fighting and fleeing, procreating and nurturing—day after day, generation after generation, millennium after millennium. And at several hundred thousand years ago, all that could have been anticipated for humans would have been a continuation along this same path, a continuation for perhaps many more millions of years, with only the subtlest of change being allowed through evolutionary means. Nothing else could have been anticipated because evolutionary constraint had never allowed a significant deviation even once, not for hominins over millions of years, and not for any other species since the beginning of life itself.

That humans did not continue along this same path is biologically extraordinary. That humans freed themselves from evolution’s behavioral and perceptual constraints is biologically radical. And that humans, in such a short period of time, managed to reconstruct their environment into the artificial dominion we live within today is nothing short of biologically shocking. Whatever sparked humanity’s radical deviation, it could not have been subtle, and it was almost certainly not evolutionary. Whatever sparked humanity's radical deviation could have only been atypical and subversive—atypical and subversive enough to smash evolution’s formidable chains.


What I would propose is this. Humans became the first species to sustain a significant percentage of autistic individuals within its population, significant enough to allow the perceptual characteristics of those individuals to begin to influence the perceptual characteristics of the population as a whole.

Without an autistic influence, it can be expected that human perception would have remained tightly and biologically constrained, just as it had been for quite some time, with each individual’s sensory focus directed almost exclusively towards the biological features contained within the surrounding environment and towards the other members of the species. Humans still experience the impact of this form of perception to this very day—it is the reason so much of our current attention is still given over to food, danger, sex and the like, and also to other people. What has changed today is that humans now also perceive so much more—symmetry, pattern, repetition, number, logic, etc.—all the structural scaffolding that underlies the artificial construction that has been accumulating all around us. But where could this supplemental form of perception have come from, how did it originate? Biologically typical humans are not prone to perceiving the non-biological structure contained within the surrounding environment, because the restrictive power of biological and conspecific perception is such that it has always relegated alternative forms of structure to the sensory background. Thus on their own, biologically typical humans do not naturally perceive underlying symmetry, pattern, repetition, number, logic, etc.

But autistic individuals do naturally perceive this underlying non-biological structure. The ironic cunning of the nature of autism, a condition that presents significant survival-and-procreative challenges, is that it also attacks evolutionary constraint right at its very core. Weakened in their degree of conspecific perception and needing somehow to organize their sensory world, autistic individuals end up evading the usual sensory constraints, and instead become naturally drawn to perceiving the non-biological structure in their surrounding environment. Furthermore, autistic individuals do more than just perceive this surrounding structure, they are also driven to recreate it, through their so-called repetitive behaviors and interests. If we could see back to the beginning, we might witness the effects of gravity being mirrored in straight lines drawn upon the ground, or the repetition of dripping water being echoed by rhythmically clapping hands—perhaps the first instances of artificial construction to be introduced into the human environment. It is of course not possible to say for certain just exactly how the first instances of autistic influence manifested within the population, a process that was likely slow and halting at first. But if we are looking for the subversive spark that sent humans cascading down this alternative perceptual path, it cannot be to the biologically typical population that we turn our gaze, a population that had been enjoined from any alternative form of perception for millions of years. Instead we must turn to the autistic population, where we find exactly the characteristic we are looking for, namely an inherent compulsion towards non-biological perception and towards artificially structured behavior, a compulsion we can witness with our own eyes today.


Although autistic individuals are almost certainly the originators of non-biological perception in humanity, it is also important to recognize that autistic influence is such that it catalyzes non-biological perception in the population as a whole. That is, given a large enough and stable enough presence of autistic individuals, the non-autistic members of the population will also over time begin to perceive underlying non-biological structure and to adopt many of the autism-inspired artificially structured behaviors.

There are two major factors driving this non-autistic adoption of autistic perceptions and behaviors. The first factor is conspecific perception. Biologically typical humans have a keen eye for noticing what other humans do. Of course when every human is biologically typical, then what each observes is the same set of restricted biological and evolutionary behaviors, and nothing generally changes over time. But if there is a significant presence of autistic individuals within the population, those individuals will be providing something entirely new to observe. Thus when an autistic individual draws a symmetrical figure upon the ground, or claps his hands with staccato, or pantomimes the motions to spark a fire, or points incessantly in the same direction as the wind, there will be other humans standing nearby and paying rapt attention, perhaps preparing themselves to imitate the behavior. Furthermore, if some of these new behaviors suggest the opportunity for better eating or for better shelter or for better sexual and nurturing result, the biologically typical humans, ever alert for survival-and-procreative advantage, will find themselves paying even more attention, with an even greater incentive to copy the behavior. The strength of conspecific perception in biologically typical humans means that autistic behaviors will seldom go unobserved. Neither will go unobserved the products of those autistic behaviors, the many varieties and instances of artificial construction.

The second major factor driving non-autistic adoption of autistic perceptions and behaviors is that the artificial construction created thereby tends to be more-or-less permanent, meaning that it can serve as an ongoing and accumulating signpost for present and future generations. For instance, a tool or weapon honed into a more symmetrical point or patterned shape becomes itself an enduring fixture in the surrounding environment, with its underlying structure now continuously on display for anyone who uses the artifact or observes its use by others. Thus over time, these examples of underlying non-biological structure begin to amass within the human world, with each generation becoming increasingly practiced at both seeing and mastering this structure. Note how different this is from the passing along of biological and evolutionary perceptions and behaviors. Very little about biological behavior is actually etched with any permanency into the surrounding environment; most biological behavior is either instinctive or is learned via the mechanism of conspecific perception, meaning that each generation essentially starts afresh, with no accretive changes over time. In contrast, the enduring nature of artificial construction means that each new generation is born into a world with a larger amount of non-biological structure than was available to previous generations, and this has the persistent impact of nudging the population to greater perception and greater mastery of this structure. We should in fact recognize the concept—this is exactly the process identified previously as the essence of human intelligence, and in the very early days of autistic influence, these first instances of increased recognition and expanding mastery of accumulating artificial construction would have marked the very beginning of the Flynn effect.

The result of these two factors is that over time overall human perception becomes more and more a blend of its two separate sources of influence. Biological perception of course remains strong, as it must for a species that still needs to survive and procreate. But alongside biological perception there now grows a new way of perceiving the world, a way that focuses more on non-biological structure and leans heavily upon accumulating artificial construction, each the consequence of autistic influence. Today, in the modern world, these two forms of perception have become so thoroughly blended that we might easily mistake them for one, and it is only in the extremely young that we still encounter a purer form of each source of influence. It is only around the age of two or three that is still relatively easy to separate the biologically typical, who are naturally fascinated with other humans and what other humans do, from the autistically atypical, who are less attentive to other humans but more comfortably engaged with the structural aspects of their surrounding world. But as each individual matures and comes under the influence of a human world mixing both autistic and non-autistic characteristics, each individual becomes increasingly dexterous with each type of perception and each type of behavior, making it more and more difficult to tease the influences apart. In today’s world, it is extremely rare to find a human adult that can be described as being purely autistic or purely non-autistic; most humans today display perceptual and behavioral characteristics that combine the traits of both, even when it remains obvious which of these traits is the more natural bent.

This might be a good time to remind ourselves that the consequence of this blended form of perception has not been trivial. No longer locked into just a biological/evolutionary way of perceiving its world, humanity has unleashed upon the planet Earth the most stunning of revolutions, a revolution on par with the formation of the chemical elements, the coming together of galactic systems, and the origination of life itself. The products of this revolution are all unprecedented: freedom from evolutionary constraint, an immense landscape of artificial construction, an ongoing growth in human intelligence—all the result of unveiling and making use of the non-biological structure contained within the surrounding environment, an ability sparked at the very moment humanity began to experience the autistic way of perceiving its surrounding world.


As intriguing as it is to consider the earliest days of autistic influence, it is still more informative to recognize that this influence has remained ongoing ever since and continues unabated through the present day. Artificial construction continues to accumulate rapidly in the current environment, human intelligence advances measurably with each new generation, and human freedom from evolutionary constraint becomes more and more established with each passing day. The same catalyst that introduced non-biological perception into the species and sparked a remarkable population-wide behavioral revolution is still driving innovation and progress in the modern world. And thus the most straightforward way to assess the role autism must be playing in this ongoing revolution is to observe autism’s impact on human perceptual and behavioral change today.

In my opinion, one of the more fruitful ways in which to observe recent autistic influence can be found in the distinction to be made between the two concepts of intelligence and genius, the latter of which has an inherent association to autism. It is commonly said that genius is the product of greater intelligence, but in fact these two concepts are not equivalent at all. From prior discussion, intelligence can be described as the ability to understand and to master the artificial construction contained within the environment, as measured by performance on an IQ exam, the contents of which serve as a proxy for environmental artificial construction. With this in mind, the phrase greater intelligence can be taken in two different ways. Within a generational cohort, a person demonstrating more mastery of the existing artificial construction, by scoring better on that cohort’s IQ exam, can be described as displaying greater intelligence than his or her peers. And in cohorts separated by time, the later generations, by mastering larger amounts of extant artificial construction—reflected in the additional complexity and variety of later IQ exams—can be described as displaying greater intelligence overall than the earlier generations. But both of these instances of greater intelligence correspond only to greater mastery of the artificial construction already contained within the human environment, they do not touch in any way upon the question of how does artificial construction get inserted into that environment. For instance, an individual could achieve a better IQ performance than any of his or her peers, and yet contribute nothing further to the environment that would boost later generations. And any generational cohort, while displaying greater overall intelligence than each prior cohort, could then in theory neglect to insert any additional artificial construction into the existing environment, effectively plateauing growth in human intelligence. Thus greater intelligence does not account for how new artificial construction gets added into the human environment. For that operation, we must turn to the word genius.

Genius does not require great intelligence. Although an ability to master existing artificial construction certainly can help, the essential requirement for adding new types of artificial construction into the surrounding environment is an ability to discern formations that do not already exist, to perceive structure that no one has perceived before. Over the course of human history, the occasions where humanity’s understanding and mastery of its surrounding environment has taken a quantum leap—control of fire, development of agriculture, the Copernican revolution, Newton’s laws of motion and gravity, evolutionary theory, the Turing machine, etc.—all these occasions have served to increase the scope and range of humanity’s non-biological perception, paving the way to massive advancement in new types of artificial construction. Genius is the correct word for describing this process. Genius opens a vista onto a previously unseen world, it breaks the existing mold, it shifts the paradigm. Genius therefore is all about perception, and in particular, all about atypical perception. And this is the reason genius has an inherent association to autism.

It is not that biologically typical individuals cannot create products of genius. As stated previously, most human adults today display combined autistic and non-autistic perceptual and behavioral characteristics, and thus a biologically typical individual would have access to the type of perception that can give rise to genius (just as, in the same way, an autistic individual would have access to the type of perception that can give rise to social success). Nonetheless, those individuals who are born autistic have something of a head start and a natural advantage when it comes to producing genius. From their very first days, autistic individuals are continuously perceiving their surroundings in a way that differs from that of most other individuals—that differs, sometimes greatly, from the existing norm—and autistic individuals must often organize their sensory world in a way that can only be described as novel. Thus atypical perception is the essence of the autistic way of being, and defying the norm is the cornerstone of autistic habit. So when a new vista is to be opened onto the surrounding environment, when the existing mold needs to be broken, when the paradigm has to shift, it can be expected that this process will occur more frequently under an autistically minded influence.

History would appear to provide evidence that this is in fact the case. Those individuals responsible for many of the more famous instances of human genius constitute what can only be described as a rather curious list: Socrates, Archimedes, Newton, Kant, Beethoven, Darwin, Dostoyevsky, Einstein, Turing, and many others. Autism of course was not yet even a concept when most of these individuals lived, and so it would be with some peril and difficulty that we might attempt to apply the term autism retroactively to any of these historical figures, an attempt made even more perilous by how how poorly we still understand autism today. Nonetheless, the biographies of these individuals have a surprisingly similar character, they are often filled with behavioral terms and descriptions that suggest a degree of separation from the human behavioral norm—eccentric, iconoclastic, awkward, misanthropic, single-minded, odd, isolated—terms and descriptions that in the twenty-first century are frequently associated with autism. This does not constitute definitive proof that autism has been at the heart of these prior instances of human genius, but it does appear to be more than mere coincidence that so many of these influential and genius-producing individuals have also possessed an assortment of autistic-like traits. And at any rate, the hypothesis can still be put to a present and future test. Over the course of the twenty-first century there will be new instances of innovation, new vistas to be opened onto the non-biological structural world, new paradigm shifts. And it will be worth some observation to see how many of these new occasions of genius come also with autism lingering somewhere nearby.

Whether it is looking for evidence of autism’s connection to genius, or whether it is attempting to assess autism’s impact on the other aspects of human endeavor, an honest and dispassionate observation of autistic individuals and their influence upon the human species would be certainly worth some merit. And yet the greatest current obstacle to actually making these observations is the autism research community itself, which in fact has made very few attempts at such observations over the many years. The autism research community has been too busy, too busy to take time to understand autistic individuals for who they actually are, too busy treating autism as a medical condition, even to the point of cure and eradication. This is the ultimate irony in my opinion, since in my way of perceiving the situation, the entire notion of research itself would be utterly inconceivable without the presence of autistic individuals and the legacy of autistic influence.


The one remaining question is, why humans? Why has the species Homo sapiens gained a significant presence of autistic individuals within its population, thereby realizing the impact of that presence, when it would appear no other species has ever experienced a similar circumstance? For that matter, how did humans first gain their own autistic presence, given that such presence seemed to be lacking for quite some time? This is not an easy question to answer, in part because we still do not understand exactly what it is that produces autism. The little that we do know suggests there is a hereditary component to the condition, but whatever the genetic underpinning is, it appears to be general and not at all specific—there is no such thing as an autism gene. So trying to figure out why autism has taken hold in Homo sapiens, but in no other species, feels somewhat akin to stumbling about in the dark.

Here would be my suggestion, although I freely admit it to be speculative. It begins with the notion of conspecific distance, a theoretical measure of the amount of separation between two organisms with respect to their ability to achieve conspecific perception for each other. That is, two organisms that have a large conspecific distance between them would also likely have no mutual conspecific perception, whereas two organisms who have a short conspecific distance between them would probably experience a strong degree of conspecific perception. Although there are perhaps many different traits that could contribute to increasing conspecific distance—blindness or deafness in one of the organisms, for instance—I suspect the predominant influence on conspecific distance is the amount of similarity or dissimilarity in each organism’s respective genetic makeup. A lion and a leopard, for example, because of their genetic dissimilarity, would have a large amount of conspecific distance between them and therefore no mutual conspecific perception, whereas a lion and a lion would have a small amount of conspecific distance, because the two lions are nearly genetically alike.

But note that even within the same species, there is still going to be a certain amount of conspecific distance between all the members of the population—it is almost never the case that two organisms are genetically the same. So when it comes to achieving a strong degree of conspecific perception, such as that commonly experienced within a given species, a certain amount of conspecific distance can apparently be tolerated. But if conspecific perception remains strong when the conspecific distance is relatively small and yet becomes nonexistent when the conspecific distance becomes rather large, this implies that somewhere in between can be found a threshold, an amount of conspecific distance that goes just beyond the toleration limit and begins to produce significant impact upon the ability to achieve conspecific perception. And if we consider the circumstance of an organism being genetically different enough from the other members of its own species to find itself somewhere near or on the other side of that distance threshold, then the consequences are going to be predictable. Such an organism would almost certainly have a weakened sense of conspecific perception relative to the other members of its own species, and this is precisely the circumstance we identified as the primary characteristic of autism.

Assuming that the above description is accurate, it also strongly implies that autism is not unique to humans. It can be expected that any species would at times, due to genetic churn, have members within its population that are conspecifically distant from the others, even to the point of inducing autistic characteristics. Thus the question becomes not how does autism get introduced into a population—this would appear to be possible for almost any species—but instead, how is it that autism can take hold within a population, how does it remain persistent, so that a significant autistic presence can be maintained over time. Autism presents an assortment of survival-and-procreative challenges. Having a weakened sense of conspecific perception means that an organism would have diminished ability to participate successfully in many crucial population activities, activities such as group defense, group hunting, group observation and group learning. Furthermore, assuming that survival still remains possible despite these many handicaps, an organism with a weakened sense of conspecific perception would then face increased challenges in making a successful sexual connection, decreasing the odds for procreation. Thus whatever genetic makeup is responsible for an organism’s increased conspecific distance from the other members of the species, the resulting weakening of conspecific perception makes it extremely difficult, if not downright impossible, to propagate those traits. This would explain why autism, although individually possible within almost any species population, nonetheless has almost no chance of obtaining significant and persistent presence within that population.

And this brings us back to the original question—why humans? Why has it been that only in humans, and only quite recently, that autism has managed to gain for itself a significant and persistent presence?

Most animal species produce a large number of offspring. It is a common evolutionary mechanism that each generation will produce a large brood, of which only a small fraction will survive and go on to propagate the next generation. Thus biologically speaking, for most species, offspring are cheap—their generational value is not with any one individual but is instead with the collective potential of the cohort as a whole. If any one organism finds itself facing an increased survival-and-procreative challenge, there is no population incentive to provide extra care and attention to help that organism along. If it fails to survive and procreate, then so be it, this is nothing more than the expected evolutionary outcome, as it is for so many others.

Also, most types of offspring are born or hatched near the end of their gestational needs, and come into this world fully ready, or nearly so, to begin fending for themselves. Thus for most species, relatively little investment is made in the early rearing of helpless young, dampening the loss to be experienced when any one of these offspring turns out to be less viable. So here too, there exists no population incentive to come to the extra aid of any biologically atypical member.

But for hominins the situation is quite different. For hominins, biologically speaking, offspring are expensive, and they have become more expensive over time. Humans generally give birth to only one child at a time, and the total number is limited to only about a dozen over the course of a female’s lifespan. So there is already additional incentive to provide extra care and attention to each individual—any one loss can be significant. And furthermore, because of the transition to bipedalism and the resulting narrowing of the birth canal, human children are born quite early relative to their gestational needs, and they come into this world quite helpless, showing first indications of being able to fend for themselves only after the first year or two. Thus humans make considerable investment in the early rearing of their helpless young, an investment not to be let go of lightly. For these reasons, a Homo sapiens child is more likely to be provided with extra support and care so that it might survive and become a participating member of the population, and this remains true no matter what that child’s particular situation might happen to be, including the possibility of finding itself conspecifically distanced from the others. It may be that it has been this extra support and care that has provided the initial boost to allow autism to gain its human foothold and to begin consistently propagating its traits. That initial foothold is what is crucial. Once the initial foothold has been achieved, continuation of autism becomes easier and easier over time. One of the more obvious side effects of increased artificial construction is that survival and procreation increases greatly for the entire population, so much so that it has vaulted the human count from maybe a hundred thousand not that long ago to a whopping eight billion today. Such an increase in overall survival-and-procreative success helps perpetuate nearly every subpopulation to be found within the species, including the autistic subpopulation.

Whether it has happened by the mechanism as outlined above, or whether it has occurred by some other process, the one thing that is not in doubt is that autistic individuals now constitute a significant and ongoing presence within the human population—at least two percent according to the most recent autism prevalence studies. And there is no reason to think that autistic presence has not been near or at that level for quite some time, meaning there has been ample opportunity over the years for autistic individuals to convey their atypical influence to the remainder of the population.


To summarize the road we have traveled:

It began with the observation that the human species is extraordinary. Using Big History for context and perspective, we explored just how unprecedented and large scale the human transformation has been, leading to the remarkable circumstances humanity finds itself in today. But we also noted that much about the human transformation has remained insufficiently explained, and we dubbed these unanswered questions the riddle of humanity.

To begin examining these unanswered questions, we investigated first the concept of biological evolution, the process cited most often when attempting to explain the human transformation. But in fact we found the reality to be just the opposite, that instead of undergoing alteration to fit a given environment, humanity has reconfigured the evolutionary process, making use of artificial construction to mutate the surroundings to better fit the species’ need—a unique and radical instance of evolution being turned inside out.

Next we studied artificial construction in greater detail, linking it via the contents of an IQ exam to the topic of human intelligence. A historical investigation further revealed that human intelligence has been consistently increasing as a consequence of the growth in environmental artificial construction, meaning that the Flynn effect has been with humanity for a very long time and is not the result of any neurological alteration, but is instead due entirely to the accumulative environmental construction of intelligence.

We then returned to the question of what has sparked the events of the human transformation—what is it that has catalyzed artificial construction, freedom from evolutionary constraint, and the growth in human intelligence. It was proposed that the answer to this question is the condition known as autism. Because autism is a relatively new concept to humanity and not yet well understood, we took some time to explore the condition more deeply. Characterizing autistic individuals as possessing a weakened sense of conspecific perception, weak enough to compel such individuals to adopt a non-biological form of perception to organize their otherwise chaotic sensory world, we settled on this description as being the true nature of autism.

Finally, we proposed that the significant presence of autistic individuals within the human population has had the impact of bringing non-biological perception to the species as a whole, thereby unleashing all the unprecedented consequences of the human transformation. It was further suggested that the validity of this proposal could be assessed by taking careful observation of the impact autistic individuals have upon human perception and human behavior today. These proposals have been offered in the hope of shedding light on the riddle of humanity.

The Nature of Autism

4. The Nature of Autism

Our description and expanded definition of intelligence marks it as the most fundamental human quality underlying the human transformation. The intimate linkage of intelligence to artificial environmental reconstruction, and to the human behavioral responsiveness to that reconstruction, means that the human transformation is in essence the equivalent of the growth in human intelligence. Other human qualities, such as language skills or collective learning, are important because of their leveraging effect, but they are not fundamental. These other qualities are themselves built up out of the constructed artifacts contained within or introduced into the environment, and are thus less basic to the transformative process than intelligence itself. Intelligence essentially describes the process, and thus human intelligence, correctly defined, lies at the core of the human transformation.

Nonetheless, intelligence does not explain why there has been a human transformation—intelligence is simply part of the description and does not serve as its own cause. And nothing that has been said so far gives an indication of what has prompted humanity to head down this transformative intelligence path, and given that Earth’s long biological history has not been witness to any similar transformation before humanity came along, it would seem there must be something unique that has spurred this species to head off in this unusual direction. Intelligence therefore is the consequence of something, it is a resultant effect, and thus to discover what has catalyzed, and continues to catalyze, human intelligence and the human transformation, we are going to need to search in an entirely different direction.

To summarize what has been discussed so far, recall first that humans were once pure animals, with the same restrictive survival-and-procreative focus that is experienced by every organism that falls under evolution’s domain. As with the other animals, human perception was once tightly constrained, targeted almost entirely towards objects such as food, water, rivals, sexual targets and conspecifics, and thus human perception would have been almost entirely blinded to objects and concepts not serving immediate survival-and-procreative demand. But sometime within the last few hundred thousand years, and accelerating beginning around fifty to one hundred thousand years ago, humans began to break free of evolution’s constraints, by turning evolution’s process inside out and by reconstructing the human surroundings entirely for human benefit. These reconstructions can be characterized almost entirely by the word artificial, they rely heavily upon the structural concepts of pattern, symmetry, repetition, logic, number and form. If you look deeply into any human artifact—a building, a word, a computer chip—what you will discover is an innovative use of pattern, structure and form not typically seen in the biological world. Humans can now make these innovations because humans now perceive the structure that underlies them; humans have become adept at visualizing their world in a way that goes beyond just the biological and the evolutionary, that goes beyond just a restrictive focus on food, water, rivals, etc. So the question to be asked is, what has sparked this broadened perceptual awareness? Is there some feature, some characteristic, unique to the human population and observable within that population, that has prompted humanity to enlarge its perceptual boundaries, to break free of evolution’s perceptual constraints, to see much further than just survival-and-procreative demand?

The answer to that question is yes. There is indeed an observable and significantly present feature within the human population that has had the impact, and continues to have the impact, of broadening human perception. Furthermore, this feature’s perceptual characteristics are exactly those one would expect in accounting for the characteristics of the human transformation, namely a heightened awareness of underlying pattern, structure and form, accompanied by a diminished awareness of the survival-and-procreative world. This feature can be denoted with just one word, but I hesitate to mention that word. I suspect if every word in the English language were to be ranked in the order of its likelihood for being the underlying impetus behind the human transformation, nearly everyone would put this word somewhere near the bottom of their list. It is a word that is poorly understood. It is a word that has been mostly mischaracterized. So our first order of business will be to examine this word more carefully, to dig more deeply into its true nature, and to discover why this word is the key for explaining the perceptual changes that have been catalyzing the human transformation.

That one word is autism.


Autism as a word did not come into existence until the twentieth century. It was first used in the early 1900s by the German psychiatrist Eugene Bleuler in describing the more withdrawn characteristics of schizophrenic patients. Then nearly simultaneously in the 1940s, American psychiatrist Leo Kanner and Austrian pediatrician Hans Asperger employed the adjective autistic in their published case studies of children who were displaying a distinct set of behavioral features—namely language peculiarities, social difficulties, and obsessive engagement with unusual activities and interests. This set of behavioral features became the basis for the definition of what was thereafter recognized to be a distinct and lifelong condition, the condition now known as autism.

Although the case studies of Kanner and Asperger did include instances where the prognosis and outcome were not all that dire, during the 1950s and 1960s autism was recognized, studied and regarded almost invariably as a devastating medical condition. Outcomes were assumed to be poor, with institutionalization often regarded as inevitable, and treatments could be draconian. Autism at that time was assumed to be an extremely rare occurrence, with prevalence estimates running as low as one in ten thousand (0.01%).

These estimates would change greatly throughout the final three decades of the twentieth century, with autism becoming more and more frequently recognized and diagnosed. By the year 2000, prevalence studies were estimating that the incidence of autism was somewhere around 1 in 150 (0.67%). The main driver in this increased recognition of autism was a growing awareness that not every instance had to be severe and not every outcome had to be poor. Children were being diagnosed as having all the telltale characteristics of autism but with those characteristics ranging widely in both detail and intensity, and often easing, sometimes dramatically, with time. Terms such as high-functioning autism and Asperger’s Syndrome were invented to delineate the more promising cases from those considered to be more “classic,” although the distinction between these terms was never clearly defined. Indeed it was a confusing era for autism, with large disagreement over the meaning of the condition. The increased prevalence, combined with a lingering attitude that autism was something to be regarded as both medical and tragic, induced general fear that autism had become an epidemic within the population. Funding and research were exponentially increased, targeted almost always towards discovering both a cause and a cure. At the same time, countering voices were growing louder—including voices from autistic individuals—saying that autism was being unfairly demonized and grossly misunderstood.

Since the year 2000, attitudes and prevalence have continued to undergo major revision. Some consensus has formed around the notion that autism should be described as a spectrum, meaning that although every autistic individual exhibits to an observable degree the defining features of the condition, there is an extremely broad range of variation in both presentation and outcome. Some autistic individuals will experience more intensely the characteristics associated with autism, and will struggle to achieve independent lives, although this outcome still appears to be relatively rare. Many autistic individuals will manage to achieve some level of acclimation to their condition and will become participating members within the population, sometimes with additional support and sometimes with complete independence. There are now many examples of autistic individuals having succeeded in college, having gone on to marry and to raise families, having gained successful careers, and so on. Recently there has even been a movement in some industries, such as computer software development, to actively seek out autistic individuals for the work value of their particular characteristics. Recent prevalence studies have indicated that nearly 1 in 50 children (2.0%) are being identified as autistic by the age of eight. This high level of prevalence, combined with a growing recognition that many autistic individuals lead successful and productive lives, has helped bolster an understanding that autism likely did not spring up out of nowhere during the twentieth century, but instead that autistic individuals have been a significant presence within the human population for quite some time. Individuals once commonly described as quirky, eccentric, isolated, etc., they are now being more frequently recognized as autistic.


Despite these ongoing changes in both prevalence estimates and how autism is being generally regarded, the medical and academic communities still seem to be struggling to catch up. Research and funding have continued to be focused almost exclusively on autism as a medical condition, with treatment and cure still frequently promulgated as the ultimate goal. These attempts to uncover the medical root cause of autism have branched off into several different avenues of pursuit. One line of research has focused on autism as being a hereditary disorder, a hypothesis suggested by the fact that identical twin and other family studies have indicated a genetic underpinning for the condition. A second line of research has targeted autism as a neurological aberration, a thesis being tested through an assortment of neuroimaging studies, mostly centered around detecting atypical brain signatures in autistic individuals. Finally, autism as a metabolic condition has also received a great deal of attention and effort, as have theories suggesting a variety of environmental insults, with everything from vaccines to highway pollution being put forth as the primary trigger of disease.

The persistence of these efforts is reflected in the growing autism research literature, which has expanded by at least an order of magnitude in the last two decades alone. A recent listing of such efforts would include the following titles: Autism spectrum disorder symptom expression in individuals with 3q29 deletion syndrome; Cortical thickness abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder; and Metabolomic Signatures of Autism Spectrum Disorder. These and other representative articles demonstrate the degree to which the current research continues to focus on genetics, neurons and metabolic pathways, as well as on the pursuit of treatment and cure. But there is a growing irony haunting these many efforts, an irony that can be recognized by considering a sampling of research articles from the early 2000s: Examination of AVPR1a as an autism susceptibility gene, Neuroanatomic variation in monozygotic twin pairs discordant for the narrow phenotype for autism, and Mercury exposure in children with autistic spectrum disorder. That is to say, not much has changed in autism research over the last several decades, other than a great expansion in volume and a constant shifting of the targeted culprits. Each year new candidate genes, new targeted neural pathways, new metabolic mechanisms and new environmental triggers are put forth with great fanfare and considerable promise, which are then followed by years in which their mention gradually declines. New therapies and new drugs are frequently introduced and promoted, but then fail to deliver any significant results in any unbiased trial. Watching this futile cycle play out again and again, year after year, decade after decade, one eventually gets the sense that when it comes to autism, the medical and academic communities are essentially spinning their wheels. And if there is to be any conclusion drawn from the autism research to date, it is that we have every reason to suspect that autism is not a medical condition.


In my opinion, one of the more effective ways to achieve a greater understanding of autism is to begin by exploring what it means to be non-autistic, which is to say, what it means to be biologically typical. This is not exactly an unfamiliar topic to this discussion, because in a certain sense humans were most biologically typical when they were in the state of being pure animals, with both their behaviors and their perceptions shaped almost exclusively by survival-and-procreative demand; the biological norm is to have all of one’s effort and attention directed towards the immediate essentials—food, water, rivals, sex, etc. Of course this biologically pure state no longer entirely pertains for modern humans, including non-autistic modern humans, but it is nonetheless important to recognize how the carryover from this animal past continues to influence behavior and perception in modern times. For most humans today, despite finding themselves nearly fully immersed in an artificially constructed environment, and despite having nearly all their biological needs easily and abundantly met, nonetheless still find themselves giving a great deal of attention and effort to the familiar targets—food, water, rivals, sex, etc. Many of us can confirm this proclivity simply by examining our own thoughts and actions, and in addition, a general look at some of the more popular human activities and interests will further reveal the extent to which humans have remained strongly preoccupied with their more primitive and natural selves. Everything from soap operas to scatological humor to crosstown sports rivalries betrays the degree to which humans have continued to be fascinated with the animal aspect of humankind. Of course there is nothing surprising or maladjusted about any of these tendencies, they are in fact entirely to be expected. They are the natural consequence of humans being not all that far removed from a former purely animal state.

One of the more intriguing components of humanity's carryover from its animal past is the notion of conspecific perception. Conspecific perception is the natural tendency for organisms to possess a heightened perceptual awareness for the other members of their own species. For instance, lions perceive first and foremost other lions, honeybees perceive first and foremost other honeybees, and so on. And of course humans perceive first and foremost other humans. When one walks into a conference room, despite one’s visual field being almost entirely filled with various non-human artifacts, one’s attention is nonetheless drawn immediately and naturally to the other humans already in the room. This intensified intra-species recognition is evolutionarily fundamental and essential for a number of reasons. First, successful mating requires a physical connection with another member of the same species, an occurrence that would be haphazard at best without an enhanced perception for one’s own kind. Also, the rearing of young would be utterly ineffective if either parent or offspring could not easily identify and perceptually foreground the other—imagine the consequences of a mother unable to distinguish her own brood from the broods of other species, or a litter unable to discern and to imitate its elders. In addition, many species coalesce into physical and geographical groups for warmth, for effective pack hunting, for more tenacious defense, and so on, with these groupings themselves the evidence of how each member is more greatly attuned to the presence and activities of the others in the species. Conspecific perception is crucial to successful survival and procreation, so much so that it should probably be included within the definition of what it means to be a species.

In humans, conspecific perception is quite strong, as would be the case for almost any species considered to be social, and the strength of this human form of conspecific perception is most apparent when considering the developmental activities of the very young. Human newborns come into this world quite early and quite helpless, and their first year or two of development is essentially an ongoing scramble to gain a functional foothold. Careful observation of these early years reveals the extent to which human newborns both rely upon and are deeply attuned to the presence and activities of other humans: a mother’s soothing voice, a father’s reassuring touch, the smiling gestures of familiar faces. Nearly every child responds immediately, favorably and naturally to these intra-species impressions. And note how critical this process must be in giving the newborn his or her sensory grounding, because without a strong dose of conspecific perception the surrounding environment would most likely emerge as nothing but a cacophony of random sensations: a wild mix of colors and shapes in the visual field, a buzzing range of tones and intensities inside the ear, a chaos of temperatures and impressions upon the skin, and a kaleidoscope of haphazard tastes and smells. Which of these impressions are to be latched upon as important, and which of them can be ignored? Which sensations should be promoted to the perceptual foreground, and which can be discarded into the undiscerned remainder? It is primarily conspecific perception that provides the organizational grounding around which a newborn’s sensory world can be arranged. From out of the chaos of countless sensations there emerges a human-forward world: human faces, human laughter, human touch, human smells, human activities. Everything associable to the human species gets a natural preference in the newborn’s sensory field, thereby guaranteeing that the newborn’s burgeoning perceptual world will become first and foremost a human world.


In addition to conspecific perception’s primary impact of providing sensory and developmental grounding, an impact shared in common with almost every other animal species, conspecific perception in humans now also serves a secondary purpose, that of providing a species-wide awareness of the new features and behaviors being brought forth by the human transformation. When one thinks of the many structured artifacts and exploits that now dominate the human landscape—a rattle, a book, a university lecture—one might wonder at first why humans would give any attention at all to these artificial impressions, given that there is no natural incentive to do so. These objects and activities are not food, they are not water, they are not sex, etc., and thus in the natural world, in the world of pure animals, these objects and activities would seem destined to become part of the undistinguished perceptual background. But of course the reason these strange artifacts and behaviors end up garnering a great deal of human attention, including the attention of humans of a very young age, is that these artifacts and behaviors have become intimately connected to the human species itself. Humans touch these artifacts, humans point at these artifacts, humans put these artifacts into other people’s hands. Thus once an artificial object or behavior has gained sufficient foothold to become part of the fabric of human experience, that object or behavior gets promoted to the human perceptual foreground, because conspecific perception gives humans the natural inclination to pay attention to what other humans do.

Thus a large part of what it means to be biologically typical is to participate in an immense and shared perceptual network of human-centric features and behaviors, some of which date back to the species’ purely animal past, and some of which correspond to the changes of modern times. Humans eat what other humans eat, humans fear what other humans fear, and humans gather where other humans are. And furthermore, when one human makes a gesture, or utters a word, or scribbles something down, there will be other humans standing nearby and paying the closest of attention, supporting the entire range of constructed artifacts and behaviors that fall under the heading of human language. And when one human points to the heavenly bodies, or narrates the tribe's origin story, or demonstrates the workings of the newest innovation, there will be other humans avidly watching and listening, reinforcing a broad array of structured behaviors that constitute collective learning. The continuity of human behavior, as well as the continuity of the human environment—including those activities and features arising out of the human transformation—all ride on a sea of conspecific perception, the natural glue holding together the species and its actions.

Since conspecific perception is so clearly crucial to both human development and to the species-wide awareness supporting the many features defining the human transformation, it raises an interesting question about what would happen if a member of the species did not possess a strong sense of conspecific perception. What would be the developmental consequence of a newborn coming into this world less able than other humans to perceptually foreground the human aspects of the surrounding environment, and what would be the overall ramifications of an individual not able to obtain his or her sensory grounding from a human-forward world? This is not really a theoretical question, because I believe we already know the answer. Any member of the human species possessing a weakened sense of conspecific perception, any human less able than other humans to perceptually foreground the human aspects of the surrounding environment, any individual unable to obtain his or her sensory grounding from a human-forward world, that individual would be most accurately described as autistic.


One of the chief defining characteristics of autism is that autistic individuals experience a broad assortment of what are usually described as social difficulties: lack of eye contact, unwillingness to participate in reciprocal play or sharing, failure to point or to follow the pointings of others, reluctance to engage in small talk and in other forms of social interaction, etc. The autism research literature has tended to blame these difficulties on presumed deficits in some proposed biological or neurological mechanism, but I would suggest that these long-standing conjectures are incorrect on two different fronts—one, these conjectures are mischaracterizing the conduct, and two, they are understating its cause. When one observes carefully the actual activities of autistic individuals, and especially the activities of very young autistic individuals, it becomes quickly apparent that these individuals are to a significant degree disengaged from the other humans around them. Whereas most children will readily interact with other people—laugh with them, play with them, follow enthusiastically their every gesture, touch and sound—autistic individuals by contrast seem largely unattuned to the presence of other humans. Autistic toddlers often do not respond to their name being called, and can be seen as being reluctant and awkward with such things as hugs and coos. Young autistic children attend less to other people than to favorite objects and interests. Autistic adolescents seldom pursue the range of friendships and relationships that other adolescents usually do. And even autistic adults, many of whom have become reasonably acclimated by then to various social customs and expectations, will nonetheless often describe their inner experience as one of extreme isolation and alienation. Thus autistic individuals are not demonstrating specific social deficits so much as they are demonstrating a broad-scale disinclination towards the members of their own species, and it is this broad-scale disinclination that accounts for the various social difficulties. But a broad-scale disinclination towards the members of one’s own species is the same thing as saying that an autistic individual is experiencing a weakened sense of conspecific perception. Unlike biologically typical humans, who will quite naturally perceive first and foremost other people, autistic individuals do not possess this natural tendency, and thus theirs is not first and foremost a human world.

That autistic individuals are dealing with a weakened sense of conspecific perception is evidenced also by the frequency with which these individuals experience an assortment of sensory issues. Many autistic individuals report a wide and non-specific range of sensory symptoms: for instance, being overwhelmed by the intensity of various textures, noises and smells (hypersensitivity); or being oblivious to extreme sensations, such as a shouted name or the sudden onset of hot and cold (hyposensitivity); or a commingling of the senses, such as “seeing” tones or “feeling” colors (synesthesia). The motleyness of these sensory symptoms suggests that they are not the result of any specific physical defect but are instead the consequence of a more general difficulty in obtaining sensory grounding. Biologically typical children rely upon conspecific perception to organize their otherwise chaotic array of sensory impressions, favoring and foregrounding those experiences that are in some way connected to the human species. But autistic individuals, not very aware of other people and not naturally favoring human-associated impressions, find themselves dealing with what must seem to be an overwhelming cascade of random and chaotic sensations, with no clearcut means for achieving sensory organization or cementing a sensory grounding, resulting in the many observed sensory issues as well as in a delay of perceptual development.

This weakness in conspecific perception can vary greatly from individual to individual, and this is perhaps one of the reasons that autism presents as a spectrum. Some autistic individuals appear to be almost entirely lacking in perceptual attachment to human presence, and these individuals can be seen as facing the greater challenge in achieving developmental gains. Other autistic individuals do seem to retain some level of connection and perceptual awareness for other humans—albeit much less than that of their biologically typical peers—and these individuals would appear to have the better chance of reaching independence and well-being. But despite the variation, there is nonetheless a threshold that would appear to be critical in determining the autistic/non-autistic divide. Any human individual with a strong enough sense of conspecific perception to be able to make use of that perception to achieve his or her sensory grounding, that individual is to be classified as non-autistic. Such an individual will strongly attach to the human species itself and will begin to see the surrounding world in much the same way as other humans do. And in the modern world, such an individual will be able to leverage this human connection into the realms of language and collective learning, where conspecific perception plays such an important role, and the individual will by these means begin to easily follow the same developmental path being traveled by the large majority of the population.

In contrast, any individual with a sense of conspecific perception so weak as to be unable to use that perception to achieve a strong sensory grounding, that individual is to be classified as autistic. Such individuals will find themselves dealing at first with something akin to a sensory chaos, since there will be few prominent features, such as other humans, naturally standing out from the manifold of sensory impressions. Such individuals will thus be cut off from the typical form of sensory organization and will not be able to easily follow the same developmental path as their biologically typical peers. Such individuals will not be able to perceive their surrounding environment in the same way as other humans do.

And this at last gets us to the heart of the matter, the key to why autism is so critical for understanding the spark underlying the human transformation. Biologically typical humans experience a world that is organized primarily around the human species and its members, biologically typical humans perceive first and foremost a species-centric world. Autistic individuals do not primarily perceive this species-centric world, and thus what they tend to perceive is something entirely different. And that is the critical question: what exactly is it that autistic individuals tend to perceive?


Another chief defining characteristic of autism is that autistic individuals frequently engage in what are usually described as restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests. In young autistic children, examples of these behaviors and interests cover a broad range of curious activities: hand flapping, lining up toys, eating the same food for every meal, obsession with certain objects such as ceiling fans and light switches, resistance to furniture rearrangement or to changes in a geographical route, strict adherence to ritual and to order in activities such as dressing, and so on. Later on in life, autistic adolescents will commonly focus much of their time and energy on a limited set of particular interests, such as sports statistics or dinosaurs or the weather, and will often perseverate (talk constantly) about a favorite topic. Autistic adults can sometimes be seen as leveraging their interests into studies and careers, with the stereotypical target of these efforts being those activities known for their rigid structure and rules: mathematics, physics, chess, computer programming, etc. A large amount of autism treatment is aimed at suppressing these various behaviors and interests, because much of the autism research community still regards these activities as anti-productive and harmful. But in a manner ranging all the way from screaming tantrums to the most eloquent of postings placed online, autistic individuals can be observed forcefully resisting these many attempts at suppression. And indeed, when one watches carefully the so-called restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests of autistic individuals, it is hard not to come away with the impression that for such individuals these behaviors and interests are utterly necessary, as though serving an essential purpose.

That essential purpose is the obtaining of a sensory grounding. When one considers the circumstance of an autistic individual not possessing a strong sense of conspecific perception, and in particular not able to make use of conspecific perception to help with sensory organization, one recognizes that this individual is facing the most dire of outcomes. As has been described previously, unfiltered sensory impressions are apt to be experienced as both chaotic and overwhelming: the wild mix of colors and shapes in the visual field, the buzzing range of tones and intensities inside the ear, the chaos of temperatures and impressions upon the skin, and the kaleidoscope of haphazard tastes and smells. If these sensory circumstances were to remain unresolved, the autistic individual would be unable to obtain any perceptual signal from the sensory environment, and would be left with only sensory noise. In turn this would mean that the barriers to developmental progress would be set impossibly high. But most autistic individuals do not end up experiencing this dire outcome. We know that most autistic individuals do manage to make significant developmental progress, even if somewhat delayed compared to their non-autistic peers, and many autistic individuals do go on to become participating and productive members within the general population, navigating quite successfully the features of a modern human world. So these individuals have not become stuck inside a sensory chaos, and must therefore be achieving a functional degree of sensory organization. But if that sensory organization has not been built around conspecific perception, then what has it been built around?

The trick here is to recognize that I have not been exactly forthcoming by characterizing the sensory field as entirely random. On the planet Earth, the sensory field, although indeed wildly multivariate, still possesses within itself a great deal of inherent structure and form. In addition to the biological structure imparted by the evolutionary propensity towards food, water, conspecifics and the like—the structural organization that most organisms latch onto quite naturally—there is also a great deal of structure that arises from such influences as gravity, chemistry, thermodynamics, celestial cycles, etc. Trees grow tall in a straight line, mountain peaks have a particular shape, water drips in a rhythm, the moon cycles through regular phases, and of course in the modern world artificial structure can be found practically everywhere. These non-biological instances of environmental structure and form are captured in a variety of words and concepts: symmetry, pattern, repetition, logic, number. These concepts possess one characteristic in common, they are all chaos-defying features. In the sensory world, these are the elements that serve to break the background noise.

The interesting thing is, for most biological organisms, they never seem to become aware of these non-biological structural features, never become aware of the many instances of symmetry, pattern, repetition, etc. It can be surmised that the reason for this lack of awareness of non-biological structure is that it is not strictly necessary for survival and procreation. Having successfully organized their sensory experience into a biologically and conspecifically guided form of perception, and having had their fitness greatly boosted by this particular form of perception, most organisms then find themselves locked into that way of perceiving their world, remaining almost entirely blind to any other type of structure their world might happen to contain.

But for autistic individuals, less able to organize their sensory experience around the usual biological concepts—including most particularly around the notion of conspecific perception—and at risk for the dire developmental consequence that would result from a persistent sensory chaos, will latch onto any alternative means of sensory organization that happens to be available. Thus autistic individuals, unlike their biologically typical peers, will find themselves becoming directly aware of non-biological structure and form, will find themselves becoming directly aware of symmetry, pattern, repetition, logic, number, and so on. From the pressing need to resolve their potential sensory chaos, autistic individuals will begin to hone in on those environmental features that serve to break the background noise.

That autistic individuals are embracing this alternative perceptual path is most evident from their so-called restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests. These activities are not arbitrary, but indeed have a requisite quality to them—all promote and enhance the non-biological structure that an autistic individual has begun to crave. Hand flapping is rhythmic to both sight and touch, every routine is a repetition. Ceiling fans encompass both symmetrical shape and regular motion, light switches capture a logic. And note the distinction in the use of toys, for instance in a set of dolls and dishes. The biologically typical child might easily be found sharing such toys with other children, setting out perhaps the scenario of an afternoon tea party, the type of interactive play that rides so firmly upon the shoulders of conspecific perception. But the autistic child is much more likely to line up these toys, or form them into a circle or some other patterned shape, carving out yet one more instance of non-biological structure in the child’s sensory field. The restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests of autistic individuals serve an essential purpose, they bring forward the non-biological structure inherently contained in the surrounding environment, they enhance the perceptual experience of symmetry, pattern, repetition, logic, number, and so on. They bring organization to the autistic individual’s sensory world.


These days, biologically typical children chart a developmental path that begins, via the mechanism of conspecific perception, with a strong association to humankind. And because the many humans they encounter and observe along the way are also engaged with the artifacts and behaviors of a complex structural world—a world that has gone far beyond just the immediate needs of survival and procreation—these biologically typical children, naturally curious about what other humans do, soon begin to participate in this complex structural world too. Suppression of this developmental path would be predictably disastrous. If a biologically typical child were to be cut off entirely from human contact and were to be given no opportunity to leverage conspecific perception, then not only would this child be deprived of his or her preferred and natural way of perceiving the environment, this child would also be deprived of his or her most straightforward connection to the expanded structural aspects of the modern world. Fortunately, very few biologically typical children encounter such cruelty, with most today making excellent developmental progress, eventually transitioning to becoming fully contributing members of a complex human society.

By way of comparison, autistic children these days employ the same developmental path as do biologically typical children, but autistic children traverse this path in the opposite direction. Autistic children gain their sensory grounding first through an awareness and manipulation of the non-biological and structural aspects of their surrounding world. And because much of this structural world has been integrated to humankind, and because this structural world depends in many ways upon human interaction, autistic individuals—perhaps reluctantly at first—eventually progress to that awkward but helpful moment when they begin to attach themselves to the members of their own species. Here too, suppression of this developmental path would be predictably disastrous. If an autistic child were to be cut off from his or her structured behaviors and interests, then not only would this child be deprived of his or her preferred and natural way of perceiving the environment, this child would also be deprived of his or her most straightforward connection to a human-centric world. Unfortunately, far too many autistic children are actually subjected to this kind of cruelty. Many of the so-called treatments and therapies for autism are designed specifically to force the autistic child to abandon his or her preferred way of perceiving the environment, attempting to substitute instead the perceptual preferences of biologically typical children. This is such a shame. The developmental direction from autistic perception to eventual engagement with a human-centric world can be traversed successfully and yield productive results. It is the reason so many autistic individuals end up making excellent developmental progress, eventually transitioning to becoming fully contributing members of a complex human society.


This then is the nature of autism. It begins with a weakened sense of conspecific perception, weak enough that it will stymie the autistic individual from organizing his sensory experience around other humans and what other humans do. This circumstance often results in developmental delays, it often results in sensory issues. But in compensation, the autistic individual will find himself latching onto the inherent structure contained within the surrounding environment, latching onto the many examples of symmetry, pattern, repetition, logic, number, and form. And where have we heard those words before, what role have they been playing in the discussion? Were these not the words considered critical for understanding the human transformation?

Autism is a variable path—some autistic individuals will struggle mightily to make developmental progress, others will do remarkably well. But all autistic individuals have an important influence on humankind, because all help bring to the species Homo sapiens a new and revolutionary form of perception.